Hiển thị các bài đăng có nhãn memories. Hiển thị tất cả bài đăng
Hiển thị các bài đăng có nhãn memories. Hiển thị tất cả bài đăng

Thứ Năm, 28 tháng 7, 2016

Neurons Compete to Form Memories

The same populations of brain cells encode memories that occur close together in time, according to new research.



Scientists have made significant progress toward understanding how individual memories are formed, but less is known about how multiple memories interact. Researchers from the Hospital for Sick Children in Toronto and colleagues studied how memories are encoded in the amygdalas of mice. Memories formed within six hours of each other activate the same population of neurons, whereas distinct sets of brain cells encode memories formed farther apart, in a process whereby neurons compete with their neighbors, according to the team’s study, published today (July 21) in Science.

“Some memories naturally go together,” study coauthor Sheena Josselyn of the Hospital for Sick Children told The Scientist. For example, you may remember walking down the aisle at your wedding ceremony and, later, your friend having a bit too much to drink at the reception. “We’re wondering about how these memories become linked in your mind,” Josselyn said.



When the brain forms a memory, a group of neurons called an “engram” stores that information. Neurons in the lateral amygdala—a brain region involved in memory of fearful events—are thought to compete with one another to form an engram. Cells that are more excitable or have higher expression of the transcription factor CREB—which is critical for the formation of long-term memories—at the time the memory is being formed will “win” this competition and become part of a memory.

Josselyn and colleagues wondered whether two memories that are formed close together in time activated the same or distinct engrams. They trained mice to associate a specific sound with a foot shock and, later, another sound with a foot shock. When presented with the sounds alone, the animals would freeze a sign that they had formed fear memories. The mice were then sacrificed, and their brains were removed for further study. The researchers measured the expression of two genes, arc and homer1a (h1a), using fluorescent in situ hybridization. These genes label neurons that were active in the previous five minutes and 30 to 40 minutes, respectively.

The same population of neurons was active if the two memories were formed within 1.5 to six hours of each other, but not if they were formed within 18 to 24 hours of each other, Josselyn and colleagues found.



When the researchers “extinguished” an animal’s second memory by presenting the sound in the absence of a foot shock, the mouse no longer froze when it heard that sound. But it also froze less in response to hearing the first sound if the memories had been formed within six hours of each other, suggesting the two memories had become linked.

Next, Josselyn’s team manipulated the excitability of the neurons in the animals’ amygdalas using optogenetics. The researchers infected neurons in the animals’ lateral amygdalas with a herpes virus that caused the cells to express channel rhodopsin. By shining blue or red light on these neurons, the team could excite or inhibit them, respectively. The researchers attempted to artificially link two memories formed 24 hours apart by increasing the excitability of the same population of neurons before both memories were formed. When they then inhibited these cells, both memories were impaired, indicating a successful linkage.

Next the researchers tried to separate two memories formed close together in time by exciting neurons before the first memory was formed and inhibiting excitability before the formation of the second. But they found that suppressing the neurons involved in the first memory also disrupted the second memory. The researchers found similar results by increasing or decreasing the expression of CREB.
“Linking two memories was very easy, but trying to separate memories that were normally linked became very difficult,” Josselyn said.

Finally, the researchers manipulated the excitability of interneurons in the lateral amygdala, showing that neurons that successfully make it into an engram do so by competing with their neighbors. These neurons also temporarily suppress other cells from being allocated to another memory, in a winner-takes-all competition.



“This is an impressive study showing compelling evidence for a linkage between memories encoded in the lateral amygdala about similar threatening events that occur close in time,” neuroscientist Joseph LeDoux of New York University, who was not involved in the study, wrote in an email to The Scientist.

In May, researchers from the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), reported similar results in the hippocampi of mice. There was greater overlap between neurons in this area that encoded memories formed within the same day, compared with ones formed a week apart, the group showed.

“Our two papers carried out studies in two different brain regions [hippocampus and amygdala], and with very different tools, but we found very similar results as to how memories are connected across time,” UCLA’s Alcino Silva, a coauthor on the May study who has previously collaborated with Josselyn, wrote in an email to. The Scientist. “The fact that two very different brain regions share this same mechanism points to the universality of this mechanisms,” Silva added.

It makes sense that the brain would link together memories that are formed close together, Josselyn noted. The process could also explain what goes awry in conditions like schizophrenia, in which the brain abnormally links thoughts and memories.



“But before we go on to treat memory disorders, we really need to understand the basics,” she said.

Source: Tanya Lewis

YOUR INPUT IS MUCH APPRECIATED! LEAVE YOUR COMMENT BELOW.

Thứ Ba, 1 tháng 3, 2016

Neuroscientists discover ways to change our memories

This may sound a bit like science fiction, but researchers have discovered various ways to change, delete, and deploy reports in humans. To understand how this is all made possible, it is first necessary to explain how memories are formed and how each memory is stored in our brain.

With the advances in science, scientists have discovered that every memory we retain is made ​​up of multiple connections through the brain. A memory is formed when certain proteins stimulate brain cells to grow and form new connections, almost like re-wiring circuits. When this happens, the memories are procured into our brains, and will subsequently stay there as long as the functionality in the brain is sustainable.



However, a crucial aspect that many people do not fully comprehend is that long-term memories are not stable. In fact, many times that the memory was checked upon the individual, one’s memory showed signs of malleability, reset stronger, and increased vividness than before. This process is known as re-consolidation, and can explain why our memories may sometimes change slightly over time. For example, if an individual fell of a bicycle, then he/she would recall the memory as bothersome, and consequently, strengthen your memory connections with emotions of fear and sadness. Eventually, that memory of falling off the bicycle can cause and potentially instill fear into an individual. Alternatively, one can empathize with this certain kind of situation as many individuals have certainly had a traumatice experience, but over the years, have laughed over the memory instead.

The process of re-building is an essential point towards changing one’s memories.
Richard Gray of the "Telegraph" explains in a fairly understandable way: "The memories can be manipulated because they are like glass, and when they are created, they melt before solidifying. But when you recall a memory, it melt again allowing you to alter them before they solidify."



One way to change a person’s memories is to make them feel less traumatic about the memory, and avoid associating negative emotions with what had transpired. To make this possible, the Norepinephrine must be blocked. (An organic chemical that functions in the human brain and body as a hormone and neurotransmitter.)

For example, researchers in Netherlands showed that this method could be used to eradicate arachnophobia using Propranolol. (An agent produced by neurotransmitters and blocker of Norepinephrine.) For the research conducted, the scientists gathered three groups of people with arachnophobia. Two out of the three groups were shown spiders in a jar to shoot their fears. Out of those same two groups, Group A was given Propranolol and Group B was given a placebo. Lastly, Group C was given only Propranolol without showing spiders.

A few months later, all three groups were shown spiders while the scientists were measuring each group’s level of fear and types of responses. The groups who were given Propranolol without seeing spiders and a placebo(Group C & B) showed no changes in their levels of fear. Instead, the group that was shown the spiders and given Propranolol (Group A) were able to touch the spiders in the course of days. After 3 months, many of individuals of Group A still felt comfortable holding spiders, and even after a year, the fear did not return.

The same drug (Propranolol) has been tested on more occasions. In 2007, victims of post trauma were allocated into ​​2 groups as one group was given a placebo and the other Propranolol. For 10 days, the patients were asked to describe the memories of their traumatic events. The group that was given propranolol were able to recall events with less apprehension.



A similar technique was employed on mice, which makes them forget a particular sound associated with electric shocks, leaving their memories intact.


To our knowledge, humans that have not yet attempted to completely erase a memory, various ethical, although in theory the right combination of medication and exercise to remember, this is possible.

Ultimately, the most worrying facet about the research is to implement memories in people who have proven to be fairly easy. The psychologist Julia Shaw has shown that a person may remember having committed a crime that they in fact did not commit, and even recall details of the fictional event.

YOUR INPUT IS MUCH APPRECIATED! LEAVE YOUR COMMENT BELOW.

 
OUR MISSION